User:Acwwpp/sandbox

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Anterior Temporal Lobe
The anterior temporal lobe displayed in pink.
Anatomical terminology

The Anterior Temporal Lobe, often shortened to ATL, is located at the anterior (frontal) area of the temporal lobe. The ATL appears to have a critical role in semantic memory processing, with other functions relating to social cognition.[1]

Structure[edit]

There has been some difficulty delineating the exact extent of the ATL. Its complex and diverse structure has made it difficult to find a functionally homogeneous region. Some definitions have limited the area that they define as the ATL to roughly Brodmann’s area 38, otherwise known as the temporal pole[2]. The temporal pole is the most rostral area of the temporal lobe and is involved in many high-order cognitive functions. Other definitions have broadened the extent of the ATL to include the anterior half of the temporal gyri. The temporal gyri are the inferior temporal gyrus, the middle temporal gyrus, and the superior temporal gyrus[3]. Further areas include anterior sections of the fusiform gyrus and the parahippocampal gyrus.[4]

The ATL has connections with many other areas of the brain. It is connected to more posterior (rear) areas of the temporal lobe through the temporal gyri.[5] The temporal gyri are important in processing sensory information. The inferior temporal gyrus, along with the fusiform gyrus exist along the ventral visual stream, which are involved in object recognition and form representation.[6] The ventral visual stream starts from within the visual cortex in the occipital lobe and culminates in areas of the ATL. It has been proposed that there is posterior-to-anterior gradient, with more anterior regions involved in information of higher specificity.[7] Other connections include the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala.[5]

The ATL has historically been a difficult area in the brain to image using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) due to its location in the brain.[8] Its location near the sinuses and air bone interfaces causes magnetic inhomogeneities meaning that attempts to use typical echo planar fMRI sequences resulted in image distortion, loss of signal and increased noise. More recent developments have used alternative echo planar imaging fMRI sequences as well as post-image acquisition corrections to mitigate these effects.[9]

Function[edit]

Semantic Memory[edit]

There have been many attempts to generalise the function of the ATL. The leading consensus is that it is primarily involved in the processing of semantic memory.[10] Semantic memory is the knowledge we accumulate about the world, including facts and concepts.[11] Language[12], object recognition[13] and social cognition[14] are some examples of semantic information. The earliest evidence for the ATL’s role in the processing of semantic memory is through the study of semantic dementia patients. Semantic dementia is sometimes seen as a variant of the frontotemporal dementia, in which areas of the frontal lobe and the temporal lobe atrophy. It is characterised by deficits notably in semantic memory processing, while leaving other cognitive functions relatively intact.[15] Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies of the brains of those with semantic dementia have revealed tissue loss in the ATL.[16]

Anterior aspects of the temporal gyri (highlighted in green) are thought to be key aspects of the anterior temporal lobe.

In semantic representation tasks, the ventral and inferolateral ATL, made up of the anterior inferior temporal gyrus, the anterior fusiform gyrus, and the anterior superior temporal sulcus, have been found to be highly activated. fMRI results suggest that semantic representation is highly lateralised to the left hemisphere. In contrast, using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) over both the left and right hemisphere ATLs elicited a reduction in performance in synonym judgment tasks. To explain the differences in lateralisation, it has been suggested that the semantic judgment tasks require more language processing which is more lateralised towards the left hemisphere.[9] In concordance with this finding, semantic dementia patients with higher atrophy in the left hemisphere have been found to have more difficulty with naming.[17] rTMS has also shown the hemispheric nature of language processing with use over the left hemisphere of the ATL causing an inability to form irregular past tense forms of verbs. There are also differences in the processing of concrete versus abstract words. The anterior superior temporal gyrus and sulcus have been found to activate more strongly to abstract over concrete words.[18]

Semantic Hub Theories[edit]

One view of ATL function is that it operates as a hub for semantic memory. It has been theorised that it is an ‘amodal’ or domain-general hub in that it will process all kinds of semantic information. As a hub, it is where all semantic information regardless of modality can be brought together for representation.[10] The large array of deficits in semantic processing in patients with semantic dementia has provided evidence that the ATL processes a large amount, if not all kinds of semantic information.[5] The ATL as an amodal or domain-general hub has been contended, with counter theories stating that instead it and its constituent parts respond to particular forms of semantic information, more in line with domain-specificity.[14][19] Research has helped distinguish at least 34 functionally distinct parcels (brain divisions) within the ATL.[20]

One such theory of domain-specificity, the social knowledge hypothesis, suggests that the anterior temporal lobe is much more receptive to social conceptual information, rather than general conceptual information.[21] Evidence for this comes from frontotemporal dementia patients with atrophy in the right anterior superior ATL were found to have much more impairment understanding social concepts compared to other non-social abstract concepts.[22] This social knowledge hypothesis argues that three separate areas are involved in social cognition: the dorsal, ventral and superior temporal pole as well as hemispheric differences in function. The left ATL is associated with proper name retrieval, the right with feelings of familiarity. The dorsal area is associated with identifying voices and the ventral area is associated with identifying faces.[21]

An alternative domain-general theory, the graded semantic hub theory, proposes the idea that the ATL semantically processes different types of semantic information in a graded configuration. A central amodal area exists in the form of the ventrolateral ATL. The further you move away from this central area, the more specialised the form of semantic processing becomes. Research attempting to evidence this used theory of mind tasks and non-social semantic tasks to show that for both tasks the ventrolateral ATL was active. The activity of the ventrolateral ATL suggested its role as a central amodal area, involved in processing multiple modalities of semantic information.[23]

Social Cognition and Face Recognition[edit]

Social cognition has also been identified as key information processed by the ATL. Studies have also shown that frontotemporal dementias with only temporal lobe damage, excluding the frontal lobe, have led to increases in socially eccentric behaviour as well as sociopathic behaviours characterised by decreased affect and empathy.[22][24] The changes in social behaviours were mostly observed in those with right anterior temporal damage. Right hemisphere damage to the ATL has also been associated with difficulty processing emotions or processing famous faces.[25][26]

The anterior inferior temporal gyrus in the ATL has been hypothesised to be important in semantic control as well as social cognition. In a meta-analysis of fMRI studies, the left anterior inferior temporal gyrus was found to be active in four different social abilities: theory of mind, trait inference, empathy, and moral reasoning[27]. Further studies have implicated that the left ventrolateral ATL consisting of the inferior and middle temporal gyri, is active in theory of mind tasks[23]. Right ATL dysfunction due to semantic dementia has also seen theory of mind impairments.[28] The ATL has also been implicated in moral cognition[29], the idea that the brain uses emotion and reasoning to make moral decisions.[30]

Most research into face recognition have recognised two different involved regions, the fusiform face area (FFA)[31] and the occipital face area (OFA).[32] An area of the ATL has been identified which is interconnected with both the FFA and OFA. An anterior temporal face area has been proposed as a link between face recognition and facial memory.[33] In certain forms of prosopagnosia, it has been found that there was normal activity in the face areas as well as normal connectivity to the amygdala, which processes emotions. However, it was also found that there was reduced activity in the ATL, as well a decrease in connectivity between the ATL to the face areas and the amygdala.[34]

Anterior Temporal Lobectomy[edit]

An anterior temporal lobectomy is a form of treatment for drug resistant temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE). The procedure involves removing sections of the ATL.[35] Although the procedure is relatively effective at treating TLE[36], it can leave the patient with certain semantic deficits,[37] especially in naming tasks.[38] Comparing left-versus-right hemisphere lobectomies have shown differences in these deficits. Left-hemisphere anterior temporal lobectomies are highly associated with impairment in language-dependent cognitive tasks, compared with right-hemisphere lobectomies.[39] Difficulty in discrimination between semantically related objects also occurs.[40]

References[edit]

  1. ^ Bonner, Michael F.; Price, Amy R. (2013-03-06). "Where Is the Anterior Temporal Lobe and What Does It Do?". Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (10): 4213–4215. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0041-13.2013. ISSN 0270-6474. PMID 23467339.
  2. ^ McArdle, A; Edwards, R H; Jackson, M J (1991-04-01). "Effects of contractile activity on muscle damage in the dystrophin-deficient mdx mouse". Clinical science (London, England. 80 (4): 367–371. doi:10.1042/cs0800367. ISSN 1470-8736. PMID 1851074.
  3. ^ Binney, Richard J; Hoffman, Paul; Lambon Ralph, Matthew A (2016-10-01). "Mapping the Multiple Graded Contributions of the Anterior Temporal Lobe Representational Hub to Abstract and Social Concepts: Evidence from Distortion-corrected fMRI". Cerebral cortex (New York, N.Y. 26 (11): 4227–4241. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw260. ISSN 1460-2199. PMC 5066834. PMID 27600844.
  4. ^ Mion, Marco; Patterson, Karalyn; Acosta-Cabronero, Julio; Pengas, George; Izquierdo-Garcia, David; Hong, Young T.; Fryer, Tim D.; Williams, Guy B.; Hodges, John R.; Nestor, Peter J. (2010-10-15). "What the left and right anterior fusiform gyri tell us about semantic memory". Brain. 133 (11): 3256–3268. doi:10.1093/brain/awq272 – via Oxford Academic.
  5. ^ a b c Wong, Cara; Gallate, Jason (2012-04-17). "The function of the anterior temporal lobe: A review of the empirical evidence". Brain Research. 1449: 94–116. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.017. ISSN 0006-8993.
  6. ^ Simmons, W. Kyle; Barsalou, Lawrence W. (2003-05-01). "The Similarity-in-Topography Principle: Reconciling Theories of Conceptual Deficits". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 20 (3–6): 451–486. doi:10.1080/02643290342000032. ISSN 0264-3294. PMID 20957580.
  7. ^ Tyler, L. K.; Stamatakis, E. A.; Bright, P.; Acres, K.; Abdallah, S.; Rodd, J. M.; Moss, H. E. (April 2004). "Processing Objects at Different Levels of Specificity". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 16 (3): 351–362. doi:10.1162/089892904322926692. ISSN 0898-929X.
  8. ^ Visser, M.; Jefferies, E.; Lambon Ralph, M. A. (June 2010). "Semantic processing in the anterior temporal lobes: a meta-analysis of the functional neuroimaging literature". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 22 (6): 1083–1094. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21309. ISSN 1530-8898. PMID 19583477.
  9. ^ a b Binney, R. J.; Embleton, K. V.; Jefferies, E.; Parker, G. J. M.; Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010-11-01). "The Ventral and Inferolateral Aspects of the Anterior Temporal Lobe Are Crucial in Semantic Memory: Evidence from a Novel Direct Comparison of Distortion-Corrected fMRI, rTMS, and Semantic Dementia". Cerebral Cortex. 20 (11): 2728–2738. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq019. ISSN 1047-3211.
  10. ^ a b Patterson, Karalyn; Nestor, Peter J.; Rogers, Timothy T. (December 2007). "Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 8 (12): 976–987. doi:10.1038/nrn2277. ISSN 1471-0048.
  11. ^ Binder, Jeffrey R.; Desai, Rutvik H. (2011-11-01). "The neurobiology of semantic memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 15 (11): 527–536. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001. ISSN 1364-6613.
  12. ^ Kutas, Marta; Federmeier, Kara D. (2000-12-01). "Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 4 (12): 463–470. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01560-6. ISSN 1364-6613.
  13. ^ Laatu, S.; Revonsuo, A.; Jäykkä, H.; Portin, R.; Rinne, J. O. (August 2003). "Visual object recognition in early Alzheimer's disease: deficits in semantic processing: Visual object recognition in AD". Acta Neurologica Scandinavica. 108 (2): 82–89. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0404.2003.00097.x.
  14. ^ a b Olson, Ingrid R; Plotzker, Alan; Ezzyat, Youssef (2007-07-01). "The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing". Brain. 130 (Pt 7): 1718–1731. doi:10.1093/brain/awm052. ISSN 1460-2156. PMID 17392317.
  15. ^ Hodges, John R.; Patterson, Karalyn; Oxbury, Susan; Funnell, Elaine (1992). "SEMANTIC DEMENTIA: PROGRESSIVE FLUENT APHASIA WITH TEMPORAL LOBE ATROPHY". Brain. 115 (6): 1783–1806. doi:10.1093/brain/115.6.1783. ISSN 0006-8950.
  16. ^ Rosen, H. J.; Gorno–Tempini, M. L.; Goldman, W. P.; Perry, R. J.; Schuff, N.; Weiner, M.; Feiwell, R.; Kramer, J. H.; Miller, B. L. (2002-01-22). "Patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia". Neurology. 58 (2): 198–208. doi:10.1212/WNL.58.2.198. ISSN 0028-3878. PMID 11805245.
  17. ^ Ralph, M. A. Lambon; McClelland, J. L.; Patterson, K.; Galton, C. J.; Hodges, J. R. (2001-04-01). "No Right to Speak? The Relationship between Object Naming and Semantic Impairment:Neuropsychological Evidence and a Computational Model". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 13 (3): 341–356. doi:10.1162/08989290151137395. ISSN 0898-929X.
  18. ^ Holland, R.; Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010-12-01). "The Anterior Temporal Lobe Semantic Hub Is a Part of the Language Neural Network: Selective Disruption of Irregular Past Tense Verbs by rTMS". Cerebral Cortex. 20 (12): 2771–2775. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq020. ISSN 1047-3211.
  19. ^ Zahn, Roland; Moll, Jorge; Krueger, Frank; Huey, Edward D.; Garrido, Griselda; Grafman, Jordan (2007-04-10). "Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (15): 6430–6435. doi:10.1073/pnas.0607061104. ISSN 0027-8424.
  20. ^ Persichetti, Andrew S.; Denning, Joseph M.; Gotts, Stephen J.; Martin, Alex (2021-06-01). "A data-driven functional mapping of the anterior temporal lobes". The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 41 (28): 6038–6049. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0456-21.2021. ISSN 1529-2401. PMC 8276737. PMID 34083253.
  21. ^ a b Olson, Ingrid R.; McCoy, David; Klobusicky, Elizabeth; Ross, Lars A. (February 2013). "Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes: a review and theoretical framework". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 8 (2): 123–133. doi:10.1093/scan/nss119. ISSN 1749-5024. PMC 3575728. PMID 23051902.
  22. ^ a b Edwards-Lee, T; Miller, B L; Benson, D F; Cummings, J L; Russell, G L; Boone, K; Mena, I (1997-06-01). "The temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia". Brain. 120 (6): 1027–1040. doi:10.1093/brain/120.6.1027 – via Oxfrord Academic.
  23. ^ a b Balgova, Eva; Diveica, Veronica; Walbrin, Jon; Binney, Richard J. (2022-06-18). "The role of the ventrolateral anterior temporal lobes in social cognition". Human Brain Mapping. 43 (15): 4589–4608. doi:10.1002/hbm.25976. ISSN 1065-9471. PMC 9491293. PMID 35716023.
  24. ^ Rankin, K. P.; Gorno-Tempini, M. L.; Allison, S. C.; Stanley, C. M.; Glenn, S.; Weiner, M. W.; Miller, B. L. (2006-09-29). "Structural anatomy of empathy in neurodegenerative disease". Brain. 129 (11): 2945–2956. doi:10.1093/brain/awl254. ISSN 0006-8950. PMC 2562652. PMID 17008334.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  25. ^ Binney, Richard J.; Henry, Maya L.; Babiak, Miranda; Pressman, Peter S.; Santos-Santos, Miguel A.; Narvid, Jared; Mandelli, Maria Luisa; Strain, Paul J.; Miller, Bruce L.; Rankin, Katherine P.; Rosen, Howard J.; Gorno-Tempini, Maria Luisa (September 2016). "Reading words and other people: a comparison of exception word, familiar face and affect processing in the left and right temporal variants of primary progressive aphasia". Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior. 82: 147–163. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2016.05.014. ISSN 0010-9452. PMC 4969161. PMID 27389800.
  26. ^ Snowden, J. S. (2004-01-14). "Knowledge of famous faces and names in semantic dementia". Brain. 127 (4): 860–872. doi:10.1093/brain/awh099. ISSN 1460-2156.
  27. ^ Diveica, Veronica; Koldewyn, Kami; Binney, Richard J. (December 2021). "Establishing a role of the semantic control network in social cognitive processing: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies". NeuroImage. 245: 118702. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118702.
  28. ^ Irish, Muireann; Hodges, John R.; Piguet, Olivier (April 2014). "Right anterior temporal lobe dysfunction underlies theory of mind impairments in semantic dementia". Brain. 137 (4): 1241–1253. doi:10.1093/brain/awu003. ISSN 1460-2156.
  29. ^ Moll, Jorge; Zahn, Roland; de Oliveira-Souza, Ricardo; Krueger, Frank; Grafman, Jordan (October 2005). "The neural basis of human moral cognition". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 6 (10): 799–809. doi:10.1038/nrn1768. ISSN 1471-0048.
  30. ^ Baez, Sandra; García, Adolfo M.; Santamaría-García, Hernando (2017), Ibáñez, Agustín; Sedeño, Lucas; García, Adolfo M. (eds.), "Moral Cognition and Moral Emotions", Neuroscience and Social Science, Cham: Springer International Publishing, pp. 169–197, doi:10.1007/978-3-319-68421-5_8, ISBN 978-3-319-68420-8, retrieved 2022-12-04
  31. ^ Kanwisher, Nancy; McDermott, Josh; Chun, Marvin M. (1997-06-01). "The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception". The Journal of Neuroscience. 17 (11): 4302–4311. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6573547. PMID 9151747.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  32. ^ Pitcher, David; Walsh, Vincent; Duchaine, Bradley (2011-04-01). "The role of the occipital face area in the cortical face perception network". Experimental Brain Research. 209 (4): 481–493. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2579-1. ISSN 1432-1106.
  33. ^ Collins, Jessica A.; Olson, Ingrid R. (August 2014). "Beyond the FFA: The Role of the Ventral Anterior Temporal Lobes in Face Processing". Neuropsychologia. 0: 65–79. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.005. ISSN 0028-3932. PMC 4122611. PMID 24937188.
  34. ^ Avidan, Galia; Tanzer, Michal; Hadj-Bouziane, Fadila; Liu, Ning; Ungerleider, Leslie G.; Behrmann, Marlene (June 2014). "Selective dissociation between core and extended regions of the face processing network in congenital prosopagnosia". Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991). 24 (6): 1565–1578. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht007. ISSN 1460-2199. PMC 4064011. PMID 23377287.
  35. ^ "Epilepsy surgery, Temporal lobectomy, Vagus Nerve Stimulation | Cincinnati Ohio Mayfield Brain & Spine". mayfieldclinic.com. Retrieved 2022-12-04.
  36. ^ Markand, Omkar N.; Salanova, Vicenta; Whelihan, Eileen; Emsley, Christine L. (June 2000). "Health-Related Quality of Life Outcome in Medically Refractory Epilepsy Treated with Anterior Temporal Lobectomy". Epilepsia. 41 (6): 749–759. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1157.2000.tb00238.x. ISSN 0013-9580.
  37. ^ Wilkins, Arnold; Moscovitch, Morris (January 1978). "Selective impairment of semantic memory after temporal lobectomy". Neuropsychologia. 16 (1): 73–79. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(78)90044-1.
  38. ^ Stafiniak, P.; Saykin, A. J.; Sperling, M. R.; Kester, D. B.; Robinson, L. J.; O'Connor, M. J.; Gur, R. C. (1990-10-01). "Acute naming deficits following dominant temporal lobectomy: Prediction by age at 1st risk for seizures". Neurology. 40 (10): 1509–1509. doi:10.1212/WNL.40.10.1509. ISSN 0028-3878.
  39. ^ Ivnik, Robert J.; Sharbrough, Frank W.; Laws, Edward R. "Effects of anterior temporal lobectomy on cognitive function". Journal of clinical psychology. 43: 128–137.
  40. ^ Schwarz, Michael; Pauli, Elisabeth (December 2009). "Postoperative speech processing in temporal lobe epilepsy: Functional relationship between object naming, semantics and phonology". Epilepsy & Behavior. 16 (4): 629–633. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.09.016.