Wikipedia:School and university projects/Psyc3330 w10/Group19

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Priming is an effect of implicit memory, where exposure to a stimulus influences response to a subsequent stimulus. For example, when a person reads a list of words including the word table, and is later asked to complete a word starting with tab, the probability that subject answers table is higher than for non-primed people. Another example of priming involves people being shown an incomplete sketch which they are unable to identify: they are shown more of the sketch until they recognize the picture. Later they will identify the sketch at an earlier stage than was possible for them the first time.[1]

The effects of priming can be very salient and long lasting, even more so than simple recognition memory[2]. Unconscious priming effects can effect word choice on a word-stem completion test long after the words have been consciously forgotten[2].

Priming works best when the two stimuli are in the same modality. For example visual priming works best with visual cues and verbal priming works best with verbal cues. But priming also occurs between modalities,[3] or between semantically related words such as doctor and nurse.[4]

Types Of Priming[edit]

Positive vs Negative Priming[edit]

The terms positive and negative priming refer to when priming affects the speed of processing. A positive prime speeds up processing, while a negative prime lowers the speed to slower than un-primed levels[5]. Positive priming is caused by simply experiencing the stimulus[6], while negative priming is caused by experiencing the stimulus, and then ignoring it,[5]. Positive priming effects happen even if the prime is not consciously seen [6]. The effects of positive and negative priming are visible in event-related potential (ERP) readings [7].

Positive priming is thought to be caused by spreading activation[6]. This means that the first stimulus activates parts of a particular representation or association in memory just before carrying out an action or task. The representation is already partially activated when the second stimulus is encountered, so less additional activation is needed for one to become consciously aware of it.

Negative priming is more difficult to explain. Many models have been hypothesized, but currently the most widely accepted are the distractor inhibition and episodic retrieval models[5]. In the distractor inhibition model, the activation of ignored stimuli is inhibited by the brain[5]. The episodic retrieval model hypothesizes that ignored items are flagged 'do-not-respond' by the brain. Later, when the brain acts to retrieve this information, the tag causes a conflict. The time taken to resolve this conflict causes negative priming[5].

Perceptual vs Conceptual Priming[edit]

The difference between perceptual and conceptual primes is whether items with a similar form or items with a similar meaning are primed.

Perceptual priming is based on the form of the stimulus and is enhanced by the match between the early and later stimuli. Perceptual priming is sensitive to the modality and exact format of the stimulus. An example of perceptual priming is the identification of an incomplete word in a word-stem completion test. The presentation of the visual prime does not have to be perfectly consistent with later testing presentations in order to work. Studies have shown that, for example, the absolute size of the stimuli can vary and still provide significant evidence of priming [8]

Conceptual priming is based on the meaning of a stimulus and is enhanced by semantic tasks. For example, table, will show priming effects on chair, because table and chair belong to the same category[9].

Repetition[edit]

Repetition, also called direct priming is a form of positive priming. When a stimulus is experienced, it is also primed. This means that later experiences of the stimulus will be processed more quickly by the brain[10]. This effect has been found on words in the lexical decision task.

Semantic[edit]

In semantic priming, the prime and the target are from the same semantic category and share features[11]. For example, the word dog is a semantic prime for wolf, because the two are both similar animals. Semantic priming is theorized to work because of spreading neural networks[6]. When a person thinks of one item in a category, similar items are stimulated by the brain. Even if they are not words, morphemes can prime for complete words that include them[12]. An example of this would be that the morpheme 'psych' can prime for the word 'psychology'.

Associative[edit]

In associative priming, the target is a word that has a high probability of appearing with the prime, and is "associated" with it but not necessarily related in semantic features. dog is an associative prime for cat, since the words are closely associated and frequently appear together (in phrases like "raining cats and dogs")[13]. A similar effect is known as context priming. Context priming works by using a context to speed up processing for stimuli that are likely to occur in that context. A useful application of this effect is reading written text[14]. The grammar and vocabulary of the sentence provide contextual clues for words that will occur later in the sentence. These later words are processed more quickly than if they had been read alone, and the effect is greater for more difficult or uncommon words [14].

Measuring The Effects Of Priming[edit]

Priming effects can be found with many of the tests of implicit memory. Tests such as the word-stem completion task, and the word fragment completion task measure perceptual priming. In the word-stem completion task, participants are given a list of study words, and then asked to complete word "stems" consisting of 3 letters with the first word that comes to mind. A priming effect is observed when patients complete stems with words on the study list more often than with novel words. The word fragment completion task is similar, but instead of being given the stem of a word, participants are given a word with some letters missing. The lexical decision task can be used to demonstrate conceptual priming. In this task, participants are asked to determine if a given string is a word or a nonword. Priming is demonstrated when participants are quicker to respond to words that have been primed with semantically-related words, ie. faster to confirm "nurse" as a word when it is preceded by "doctor" than when it is preceded by "butter". Other evidence has been found through brain imaging and studies from brain injured patients.

Effects of Brain Injuries[edit]

Amnesia[edit]

Amnesic patients are described as those who have suffered damage to their medial temporal lobe, resulting in the impairment of explicit recollection of everyday facts and events. Priming studies on amnesic patients have varying results, depending on both the type of priming test done, as well as the phrasing of the instructions.

Amnesic patients do as well on perceptual priming tasks as healthy patients [15], however they show some difficulties completing conceptual priming tasks, depending on the specific test. For example, they perform normally on category instance production tasks, but show impaired priming on any task that involves answering general knowledge questions [16] [17].

Phrasing of the instructions associated with the test has a dramatic impact on an amnesic's ability to complete the task successfully. When performing a word-stem completion test, patients were able to successfully complete the task when asked to complete the stem using the first word that came to mind, but when explicitly asked to recall a word to complete the stem that was on the study list, patients performed at below-average levels [18].

Overall, studies from amnesic patients indicate that priming is controlled by a brain system separate from the medial temporal system that supports explicit memory.

Dementia[edit]

Patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD), the most common form of dementia, have been studied extensively as far as priming goes. Results are conflicting in some cases, but overall, AD patients show decreased priming effects on word-stem completion and free association tasks, while retaining normal performance on lexical decision tasks[19]. These results suggest that AD patients are impaired in any sort of priming task that requires semantic processing of the stimuli, while priming tasks that require visuoperceptual interpretation of stimuli are unaffected by Alzheimers.

Focal Cortical Lesions[edit]

Patient J.P., who suffered a stroke in the left medial/temporal gyrus, resulting in Auditory verbal agnosia - the inability to comprehend spoken words, but maintaining the ability to read and write, and with no effects to hearing ability. J.P. showed normal perceptual priming, but his conceptual priming ability for spoken words was, expectedly, impaired [20]. Another patient, N.G., who suffered from prosopanomia (the inability to retrieve proper names) following damage the his left temporal lobe, was unable to spontaneously provide names of persons or cities, but was able to successfully complete a word-fragment completion exercise following priming with these names. This demonstrated in-tact perceptual priming abilities [21].

Neuroanatomy[edit]

Perceptual Priming[edit]

Example alt text
The extrastriate cortex (shown in orange and red) is believed to be involved in perceptual priming

Neuroimaging studies completed using a PET scanner during a word-stem completion priming task revealed that the bilateral posterior perceptual processing areas in the extrastriate cortex of the brain showed decreased activation when completing primed word stems as compared to novel word stems [22]. The extrastriate cortex is involved in vision. This result suggests that after initial exposure to the stimulus, subsequent processing is faster and requires less neural activity, resulting in the decreased blood flow seen in the PET scan. These results have been confirmed with fMRI scans, as well as with a variety of other priming tests.

Conceptual Priming[edit]

Conceptual priming has been linked to reduced blood flow in the left prefrontal cortex [23]. The left prefrontal cortex is believed to be involved in the semantic processing of words, among other tasks [24].

The view that perceptual priming is controlled by the extrastriate cortex while conceptual priming is controlled by the left prefrontal cortex is undoubtedly an oversimplified view of the process, and current work is focused on elucidating the brain regions involved in priming in more detail. [25]

Priming In Daily Life[edit]

Priming is thought to play a large part in the systems of stereotyping[26]. This is because attention to a response increases the frequency of that response, even if the attended response is undesired[26][27]. The attention given to these response or behaviours primes them for later activation[26].

This can occur even if the subject is not conscious of the priming stimulus[26]. An example of this was done by Bargh et al. in 1996. Subjects were implicitly primed with words related to the stereotype of elderly people (example: Florida, forgetful, wrinkle). While the words did not explicitly mention speed or slowness, those who were primed with these words walked more slowly upon exiting the testing booth than those who were primed with neutral stimuli[26]. Similar effects were found with rude and polite stimuli: those primed with rude words were more likely to interrupt an investigator than those primed with neutral words, and those primed with polite words were the least likely to interrupt[26]. A Harvard study showed that something as simple as holding a hot or cold beverage before an interview could result in pleasant or negative opinion of the interviewer[28]

External Links[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ Kolb & Whishaw: Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology (2003), page 453-454, 457.
  2. ^ a b Tulving, Endel; Daniel L. Schacter; Heather A. Stark (1982). "Priming Effects in Word Fragment Completion are independent of Recognition Memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 8 (4).
  3. ^ Several researchers, for example, have used cross-modal priming to investigate syntactic deficits in individuals with damage to Broca's area of the brain. See the following:
    • Zurif, E.B., D. Swinney, P. Prather, J. Solomon and C. Bushell (1993). "An on-line analysis of syntactic processing in Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia." Brain and Language 45, 448-464.
    • Swinney, D., E. Zurif, P. Prather, and T. Love (1993). "The neurological distribution of processing operations underlying language comprehension." Manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego.
    • For an overview, see also Zurif, E.B. (1995), "Brain Regions of Relevance to Syntactic Processing." in Knowledge of Meaning: An Introduction to Semantic Theory, eds. Richard Larson and Gabriel Segal. MIT Press.
  4. ^ Friederici, Angela D.; Karsten Steinhauer; Stefan Frisch (1999). "Lexical integration: Sequential effects of syntactic and semantic information". Memory & Cognition. 27 (3): 438–453. Semantic priming refers to the finding that word recognition is typically faster when the target word (e.g., doctor) is preceded by a semantically related prime word (e.g., nurse).
  5. ^ a b c d e Mayr, Susanne; Axel Buchner (2007). "Negative Priming as a Memory Phenomenon: A review of 20 years of Negative Priming". Journal of Psychology. 215 (1).
  6. ^ a b c d Reisberg, Daniel: Cognition: Exploring the Science of the Mind (2007), page 255, 517.
  7. ^ Bentin, Shlomo; Gregory McCarthy; Charles C. Wood (1985). "Event Related Potentials, Lexical Decision and Semantic Priming". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 60.
  8. ^ Beiderman, Irving; Eric E. Cooper (1992). "Size Invariance in Visual Object Priming". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 18 (1).
  9. ^ Vaidya, Chandan L; Laura A Monti; John DE Gabrieli; Jared R Tinklenburg; Jerome A Yesevage (1999). "Dissociation between two forms of conceptual priming in Alzheimer's disease" (PDF). Neuropsychology. 13 (4).
  10. ^ Forster, Kenneth I; Chris Davis (1984). "Repetition Priming and Frequency Attenuation". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 10 (4).
  11. ^ Ludovic Ferrand and Boris New: Semantic and associative priming in the mental lexicon, found on: boris.new.googlepages.com/Semantic-final-2003.pdf
  12. ^ Marslen-Wilson, William; Lorraine Komisarjevsky Tyler; Rachelle Waksler (1994). "Morphology and Meaning in the English Mental Lexicon". Psychological Review. 215 (1).
  13. ^ Matsukawa, Junko; Joan Gay Snodgrass; Glen M. Doniger (2005). "Conceptual versus perceptual priming in incomplete picture identification". Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 34 (6).
  14. ^ a b Stanovich, Keith E.; Richard F. West (1983). "On Priming by a Sentence Context". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 112 (1).
  15. ^ Cermak, L.S.; Talbot, N.; Chandler, K.; Wolbarst, L.R. (1985). "The perceptual priming phenomenon in amnesia". Neuropsychologia. 23: 615–622.
  16. ^ Shimamura, A.P.; Squire, L.R. (1984). "Paired-associate learning and priming effects in amnesia: a neuropsychological approach". J. Exp. Psychol. 113: 556–570.
  17. ^ Blaxton, T.A. (1992). "Dissociations among memory measures in memory-impaired subjects: evidence for a processing account of memory". Mem. Cogn. 15: 549–562.
  18. ^ Graf, P.; Squire, L.R.; Mandler, G. (1984). "The information that amnesic patients do not forget". J. Exp. Psychol. 10: 164–178.
  19. ^ Carlesimo, G.A.; Oscarberman, M. (1992). "Memory deficits in Alzheimer's patients: a comprehensive review". Neuropsychology Review. 3 (2): 119–169.
  20. ^ Schacter, D.L.; McGlynn, S.M.; Milberg, W.P.; Church, B.A. (1993). "Spared priming despite impaired comprehension: implicit memory in a case of word meaning deafness". Neuropsychology. 7: 107–118.
  21. ^ Geva, A.; Moscovitch, M.; Leach, L. (1997). "Perceptual priming of proper names in young and older normal adults and a patient with prosopanomia". Neuropsychology. 11: 232–242.
  22. ^ Squire, L.R.; Ojemann, J.G.; Miezin, F.M.; Petersen, S.E.; Videen, T.O.; Raichle, M.E. (1992). "Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: a functional anatomical study of memory". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 89: 1837–1841.
  23. ^ Demb, J.B.; Desmond, J.E.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. (1995). "Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity". J. Neurosci. 15: 5870–5878.
  24. ^ Gabrieli, J.D.E.; Poldrack, R.A.; Desmond, J.E. (1998). "The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory". PNAS. 95: 906–913.
  25. ^ [1]
  26. ^ a b c d e f Bargh, John A.; Chen, Mark; Burrows, Laura (1996). "Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 71 (2).
  27. ^ (see Ironic_process_theory for a more in-depth discussion of this phenomena)
  28. ^ IJerman, Hans; Semin, Gun R. (2009). "The Thermometer of Social Relations: Mapping Social Proximity on Temperature". Psychological Science. 20 (10): 1214–1220.